what is bond between childhood and their quality education??
A.
Education To Be More’ was published
last August. It was the report of the New Zealand ‘Government’s Early Childhood
Care and Education Working Group. The report argued for enhanced equity of
access and better funding for childcare and early childhood education
institutions. Unquestionably, that’s a real need; but since parents don’t
normally send children to pre-schools until the age of three, are we missing
out on the most important years of all?
B.
A 13-years study of early childhood
development at Harvard University has shown that, by the age of three, most
children have the potential to understand about 1000 words – most of the
language they will use in ordinary conversation for the rest of their lives. Furthermore,
research has shown that while every child is born with natural curiosity, it
can be suppressed dramatically during the second and third years of life.
Researchers claim that the human personality is formed during the first three
years children learn the basic skills they will use in all their later learning
both at home and at school. Once over the age of three, children continue to
expand on existing knowledge of the world.
C.
It is generally acknowledged that
young people from poorer socio-economic backgrounds tend to do less well in our
education system. That’s observed not just in New Zealand, nut also in
Australia, Britain and America. In an attempt to overcome that educational
under achievement, a nationwide programme called ‘Headstart’ was launched in
the United States in 1965. A lot of money was poured into it. It took children
into pre-school institutions at the age of three and was supposed to help the
children of poorer families succeed in school. Despite substantial funding,
results have been disappointing. It is thought that there are two applications
for this. First, the programme began too late. Many children who entered it at
the age of three were already behind their peers in language and measurable
intelligence. Second, the parents were not involved. At the end of each day,
‘Headstart’ children returned to the same disadvantaged home environment.
D.
As a result of the growing research
evidence of the importance of the first three years of a child’s life and the
disappointing result from ‘headstart’, a pilot programme was launched in
Missouri in the US that focussed on parents as the child’s first teachers. The
‘Missouri’ programme was predicted on research showing that working with the
family, rather than bypassing the parents, is the most effective way of helping
children get off to the best possible start in life. The four-year pilot study
included 380 families who were about to have their first child and who
represented a cross-section of socio-economic status, age and family configurations.
They included single-parent and two –parent families. Families in which both
parents worked and families with either the mother or father at home. The
programme involved trained parent- educators visiting the parents’ home and
working with the parent, or parents and the child. Information on child
development, and guidance on things to look for and expect as the child grows
were provided, plus guidance in fostering the child’s intellectual, language, social
and motor-skill development. Periodic check-ups of the child’s educational and
sensory development (hearing and vision) were made to detect possible handicaps
that interfere with growth and development. Medical problems were referred to
professionals. Parent-educators made personal visits to homes and monthly group
meetings were held with other new parents to share experience and discuss
topics of interest. Parent resource centres, located in school buildings,
offered learning materials for families and facilitators for child care.
E.
At the age of three, the children who
had been involved in the ‘Missouri’ programme were evaluated alongside a
cross-section of children selected from the same range of socio-economic
backgrounds and family situations, and also a random sample of children that
age. The results were phenomenal. By the age of three, the children in the
programme were significantly more advanced in language development than their
peers, had mode greater strides in problem solving and other intellectual
skills, and were further along in social development. In fact, the average
child on the programme was performing at the level of the top 15 to 20 per cent
of their peers in such things as auditory comprehension, verbal ability and
language ability. Most important of all, the traditional measures of ‘risk’,
such as parents’ age and education or whether they were a single parent, bore
little or no relationship to the measures of achievement and language
development. Children in the programme performed equally well regardless of
socio-economic disadvantages. Child abuse was virtually eliminated. The one
factor that was found to affect the child’s development was family stress
leading to a poor quality of parent-child interaction. That interaction was not
necessarily bad in poorer families.
F.
These research findings are exciting.
There is growing evidence in New Zealand that children from poorer
socio-economic backgrounds are arriving at school less well developed and that
our school system tends to perpetuate that disadvantage. The initiative
outlined above could break that cycle of disadvantage. The concept of working
with parents in their homes, or at their place of work, contrast quite markedly
with the report of the Early Childhood Care and Education Working Group. Their
focus is on getting children and mothers access to childcare and
institutionalised early childhood education. Education from the age of three to
five is undoubtedly vital, but without a similar focus on parent education and
on the vital importance of the first three years, some evidence indicates that
it will not be enough to overcome educational inequity.
Comments
Post a Comment